Ad

Psychological Testing: Intelligence Quotient

Ad Disclosure: Some of our MentalHelp.net recommendations, including BetterHelp, are also affiliates, and as such we may receive compensation from them if you choose to purchase products or services through the links provided

An intelligence quotient, or IQ, is a score derived from one of several different standardized tests designed to assess intelligence. The term "IQ," from the German Intelligenz-Quotient, was devised by the German psychologist William Stern in 1912 as a proposed method of scoring children's intelligence tests such as those developed by Alfred Binet and Théodore Simon in the early 20th Century.[1] Lewis Terman accepted that form of scoring, expressing a score as a quotient of "mental age" and "chronological age," for his revision of the Binet-Simon test,[1] the first version of the Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scales.

Although the term "IQ" is still in common use, the scoring of modern IQ tests such as the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale is now based on standard scoring of the subject's rank order on the test item content with the median score set to 100, and a standard deviation of 15, although not all tests adhere to that assignment of 15 IQ points to each standard deviation.

Ad

IQ scores have been shown to be associated with such factors as morbidity and mortality, parental social status,[2] and, to a substantial degree, parental IQ. While the heritability of IQ has been investigated for nearly a century, controversy remains regarding the significance of heritability estimates,[3][4] and the mechanisms of inheritance are still a matter of some debate.[5]

IQ scores are used in many contexts: as predictors of educational achievement or special needs, by social scientists who study the distribution of IQ scores in populations and the relationships between IQ score and other variables, and as predictors of job performance and income.

Therapists are Standing By to Treat Your Depression, Anxiety or Other Mental Health Needs

Explore Your Options Today

Ad

The average IQ scores for many populations have been rising at an average rate of three points per decade since the early 20th century, a phenomenon called the Flynn effect. It is disputed whether these changes in scores reflect real changes in intellectual abilities, or merely methodological problems with past or present testing.

 History

 Psychometrics

Englishman Francis Galton, influenced by Darwinism, founded eugenics (and later psychometrics) to measure differences between upper and lower classes.[6] Galton argued that due to heredity, white aristocrats were intellectually superior to other humans; nurture, meanwhile, played a lesser role in intellectual capacity.[7]

 Clinical diagnostics

French psychologist Alfred Binet gave more weight to nurture, arguing that intelligence could be improved.[8] Binet and fellow French psychologist Théodore Simon later developed the Binet-Simon test for measuring intellectual development or mental age to diagnose French children in need of special assistance classes.[9] Binet-Simon tasks were diverse in order to neutralize individual differences in types of intelligence and provide a general measure.

 Intelligence testing

English psychologist Charles Spearman, however, did not believe there were different types of intelligence.[10] He devised a correlational formula to define a common intellective factor (which he called "general intelligence"), a factor that Binet argued did not exist.[11]

 Intelligence quotient (IQ)

In 1910 the eugenics movement in the USA seized on the Binet-Simon test as means to give them credibility in diagnosing mental retardation after American psychologist Henry H. Goddard published a translation of it that same year.[12] American psychologist Lewis Terman revised the Binet-Simon scale, implementing German psychologist William Stern's intelligence quotient (I.Q.) ratio of mental age to chronological age and used the resulting Stanford-Binet scale as a measure of general intelligence.[13]

 World War I: large scale testing

A team of psychologists led by eugenicist Robert Yerkes assisted the US army to rapidly assess and assign huge numbers of personnel.[14] They developed two group-administered intelligence tests for this purpose: an alpha test for literate personnel; and a beta for the illiterate. They successfully tested 1,726,000 recruits.[15] However, there was too little time for validity testing[16] and not enough staff to administer tests correctly.[17] Many officers distrusted the psychologists, accusing them of conducting research for their own purposes[17] using culturally biased tests.[18] Orders were issued that low test scores should not bar men from officer training and that a disability board should consider all discharges.[19] The outcome was that 0.5% of recruits were discharged as mentally inferior, whilst Yerkes would have preferred to have discharged 3% whose results showed a mental age of under 10.[15]

In contrast, another team of psychologists led by Walter Dill Scott defined intelligence as a diverse complex of capacities, as Binet had. They stressed an individual differences approach emphasizing environmental adjustments of mental qualities as functions.[11] Soon they developed a rating scale to classify and place enlisted men according to how they used their intelligence, not their base levels of intelligence.[18] The army was enthusiastic about Scott's classification of personnel. By the time the war ended they had already been incorporated into the military[17] and had classified 3½ million men and assigned 973,858 to technical units.[15]

 Post war to 1929

The army abolished Yerkes' team after the war but employed two psychologists to continue intelligence testing research.[17] However, a great deal of positive post war publicity on army psychological testing helped to make psychology a respected field.[20] Subsequently there was an increase in jobs and funding in psychology.[21] Group intelligence tests were developed for and became widely used in both primary and secondary schools, universities and industry.[17] But controversy followed when one of Yerkes' team, Goddard, admitted that they had been guilty of bad logic in finding that 45% of army recruits had a mental age of 12 or less.[22] In addition, there was no agreement on what the intelligence tests measured,[23][24][25] and the publication of racial differences in scores[26] led to much criticism that environmental influences on score had not been considered.[23][27]

Wechsler's innovation in scoring

The IQs of a large enough population are calculated so that they conform[28] to a normal distribution.

The modern IQ scale is a mathematical transformation of a raw score on an IQ test, based on the rank of that score in a normalization sample. This procedure was pioneered by David Wechsler. Modern scores are sometimes referred to as "deviation IQs," while older method age-specific scores are referred to as "ratio IQs."[29] IQ scales of either kind are ordinal scales and thus IQ points are not units of measurement.[30][31][32][33]

The two scoring methodologies yield similar results near the population median, but the older ratio IQs yielded far higher scores for high-scoring (gifted) children. Current IQ tests do not yield such high scores, as recent validation studies have shown that concerns about ceiling effects in identifying gifted children are exaggerated.[34] In any event error of estimation is greatest for scores above the population median[35] and likely to overestimate the true score (in the sense of formal testing theory) of a test-taker who obtains a score above the median.[36]

Since the publication of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS), almost all intelligence scales have adopted the deviation method of scoring. The use of this scoring method makes the term "intelligence quotient" an inaccurate description, mathematically speaking, but the term "IQ" still enjoys colloquial usage,[37] and is used to label the standard scoring of most cognitive ability tests currently in use.

Reliability

IQ scores can differ for the same individual even on the most sophisticated current IQ scales. (IQ score table data and pupil pseudonyms adapted from description of KABC-II norming study cited in Kaufman 2009.[38])
Pupil KABC-II WISC-III WJ-III
Asher 90 95 111
Brianna 125 110 105
Colin 100 93 101
Danica 116 127 118
Elpha 93 105 93
Fritz 106 105 105
Georgi 95 100 90
Hector 112 113 103
Imelda 104 96 97
Jose 101 99 86
Keoku 81 78 75
Leo 116 124 102

Psychometricians generally regard IQ tests as highly reliable (in the technical sense of testing theory) and clinical psychologists generally regard them as having sufficient validity for many clinical purposes. A test-taker's score on any one IQ test is surrounded by an error band that shows, to a specified degree of confidence, what the test-taker's true score (in that same technical sense) is likely to be. Test-takers can have varying scores on differing occasions of taking IQ tests and can vary in scores on different IQ tests taken at the same age.[39][40][41]

The General Intelligence Factor (g)

There are many different kinds of IQ tests using a wide variety of methods. Some tests are visual, some are verbal, some tests only use of abstract-reasoning problems, and some tests concentrate on arithmetic, spatial imagery, reading, vocabulary, memory or general knowledge.[42]

The psychologist Charles Spearman early this century made the first formal factor analysis of correlations between the tests. He found that a single common factor explained for the positive correlations among tests. This is an argument still accepted in principle by many psychometricians. Spearman named it g for "general intelligence factor." In any collections of IQ tests, by definition the test that best measures g is the one that has the highest correlations with all the others. Most of these g-loaded tests typically involve some form of abstract reasoning. Therefore Spearman and others have regarded g as the perhaps genetically determined real essence of intelligence. This is still a common but not proven view. Other factor analyses of the data with different results are possible. Some psychometricians regard g as a statistical artifact. The accepted best measure of g is Raven's Progressive Matrices which is a test of visual reasoning.[42]

Since the early twentieth century, raw scores on IQ tests have increased in most parts of the world.[43][44][45] When a new version of an IQ test is normed, the standard scoring is set so that performance at the population median results in a score of IQ 100. The phenomenon of rising raw score performance means that if test-takers are scored by a constant standard scoring rule, IQ test scores have been rising at an average rate of around three IQ points per decade. This phenomenon was named the Flynn effect in the book The Bell Curve after James R. Flynn, the author who did the most to bring this phenomenon to the attention of psychologists.[46] [47]

Researchers have been exploring the issue of whether the Flynn effect is equally strong on performance of all kinds of IQ test items, whether the effect may have ended in some developed nations, whether or not there are social subgroup differences in the effect, and what possible causes of the effect might be.[48] Flynn's observation has prompted much new research in psychology and "demolish some long-cherished beliefs, and raise a number of other interesting issues along the way."[44]

Environmental and genetic influences and malleability

Environmental influences

Environmental factors play a role in determining IQ. Proper childhood nutrition appears critical for cognitive development; malnutrition can lower IQ. For example, iodine deficiency causes a fall, in average, of 12 IQ points.[49] It is expected that average IQ in third world countries will increase dramatically if the deficiencies of iodine and other micronutrients are eradicated.

A recent study found that the FADS2 gene, along with breastfeeding, adds about seven IQ points to those with the "C" version of the gene. Those with the "G" version of the FADS2 gene see no advantage.[50][51]

Musical training in childhood may also increase IQ.[52] Recent studies have shown that training in using one's working memory may increase IQ.[53][54]

Family environment

Some studies in the developed world have shown that inherited personality traits cause non-related children raised in the same family ("adoptive siblings") to be as different as children raised in different families.[55][not in citation given][56] There are some family effects on the IQ of children, accounting for up to a quarter of the variance; however, by adulthood, this correlation approaches zero.[57] For IQ, adoption studies show that, after adolescence, adoptive siblings are no more similar in IQ than strangers (IQ correlation near zero), while full siblings show an IQ correlation of 0.6. Twin studies reinforce this pattern: monozygotic (identical) twins raised separately are highly similar in IQ (0.86), more so than dizygotic (fraternal) twins raised together (0.6) and much more than adoptive siblings.[55][not in citation given] However these results make no correction for the social and emotional effects frequently associated with adoption.

Possible bias in older studies

Stoolmiller (1999)[58] found that the range restriction of family environments that goes with adoption, that adopting families tend to be more similar on, for example, socio-economic status than the general population, suggests a possible underestimation of the role of the shared family environment in previous studies. Corrections for range correction applied to adoption studies indicate that socio-economic status could account for as much as 50% of the variance in IQ.[58] However, the effect of restriction of range on IQ for adoption studies was examined by Matt McGue and colleagues, who wrote that "restriction in range in parent disinhibitory psychopathology and family socio-economic status had no effect on adoptive-sibling correlations [in] IQ".[59]

Eric Turkheimer and colleagues (2003)[60] studied the "Massive IQ Gains in 14 Nations: What IQ Tests Really Measure"
heritability of IQ in impoverished US families. Results demonstrated that in small children the proportions of IQ variance attributable to genes and environment vary nonlinearly with socio-economic status. The models suggest that in impoverished families, 60% of the variance in early childhood IQ is accounted for by the shared family environment, and the contribution of genes is close to zero; in affluent families, the result is almost exactly the reverse.[61] They suggest that the role of shared environmental factors may have been underestimated in older studies which often only studied affluent middle class families.[62]

Maternal (fetal) environment

A meta-analysis, by Devlin and colleagues in Nature (1997),[5] of 212 previous studies evaluated an alternative model for environmental influence and found that it fits the data better than the 'family-environments' model commonly used. The shared maternal (fetal) environment effects, often assumed to be negligible, account for 20% of covariance between twins and 5% between siblings, and the effects of genes are correspondingly reduced, with two measures of heritability being less than 50%.

Bouchard and McGue reviewed the literature in 2003, arguing that Devlin's conclusions about the magnitude of heritability is not substantially different from previous reports and that their conclusions regarding prenatal effects stands in contradiction to many previous reports.[63] They write that:

Chipuer et al. and Loehlin conclude that the postnatal rather than the prenatal environment is most important. The Devlin et al. conclusion that the prenatal environment contributes to twin IQ similarity is especially remarkable given the existence of an extensive empirical literature on prenatal effects. Price (1950), in a comprehensive review published over 50 years ago, argued that almost all MZ twin prenatal effects produced differences rather than similarities. As of 1950 the literature on the topic was so large that the entire bibliography was not published. It was finally published in 1978 with an additional 260 references. At that time Price reiterated his earlier conclusion. Research subsequent to the 1978 review largely reinforces Price's hypothesis.

Dickens and Flynn model

Dickens and Flynn[64] postulate that the arguments regarding the disappearance of the shared family environment should apply equally well to groups separated in time. This is contradicted by the Flynn effect. Changes here have happened too quickly to be explained by genetic heritable adaptation. This paradox can be explained by observing that the measure "heritability" includes both a direct effect of the genotype on IQ and also indirect effects where the genotype changes the environment, in turn affecting IQ. That is, those with a higher IQ tend to seek out stimulating environments that further increase IQ. The direct effect can initially have been very small but feedback loops can create large differences in IQ. In their model an environmental stimulus can have a very large effect on IQ, even in adults, but this effect also decays over time unless the stimulus continues (the model could be adapted to include possible factors, like nutrition in early childhood, that may cause permanent effects). The Flynn effect can be explained by a generally more stimulating environment for all people. The authors suggest that programs aiming to increase IQ would be most likely to produce long-term IQ gains if they taught children how to replicate outside the program the kinds of cognitively demanding experiences that produce IQ gains while they are in the program and motivate them to persist in that replication long after they have left the program.[64][65]

Genetic influences

Various studies find the heritability of IQ between 0.4 and 0.8 in the United States;[66][67][68] that is, depending on the study, a little less than half to substantially more than half of the variation in IQ among the children studied was estimated to be due to genetic variation. Heritability measures the proportion of variation that can be attributed to genes within any measured population (however defined), and not the extent that genes contribute to intelligence. It should also be noted that the idea that genetic and environmental influences on intelligence are independent is not necessarily accepted, or as Jeremy Freese says "The lucidity of Lewontin's arguments has historically proven no match for the allure of overly simple characterizations of outcomes as being x% due to genes and (1 – x)% not due to genes". That means that genes affect environment and environment affects genes.[69][70][71][72][73][74] A heritability in the range of 0.4 to 0.8 implies that IQ is "substantially" heritable, that is, some substantial part of the variation within a population is caused at least in part by genes.

The effect of restriction of range on IQ was examined by Matt McGue and colleagues, who wrote that "restriction in range in parent disinhibitory psychopathology and family SES had no effect on adoptive-sibling correlations ... IQ."[59] On the other hand, a 2003 study by Eric Turkheimer, Andreana Haley, Mary Waldron, Brian D'Onofrio, Irving I. Gottesman demonstrated that the proportions of IQ variance attributable to genes and environment vary with socioeconomic status. They found that in impoverished families, 60% of the variance in IQ "in a sample of 7-year-old twins" is accounted for by the shared environment, and the contribution of genes was close to zero.[75]

It is reasonable to expect that genetic influences on traits like IQ should become less important as one gains experiences with age. However, the opposite occurs.[76] Heritability measures in infancy are as low as 20%, around 40% in middle childhood, and as high as 80% in adulthood.[76] The American Psychological Association's 1995 task force on "Intelligence: Knowns and Unknowns" concluded that within the white population the heritability of IQ is "around .75." The Minnesota Study of Twins Reared Apart, a multiyear study of 100 sets of reared-apart twins which was started in 1979, concluded that about 70% of the variance in IQ was found to be associated with genetic variation. Some of the correlation of IQs of twins may be a result of the effect of the maternal environment before birth, shedding some light on why IQ correlation between twins reared apart is so robust.[5] There are a number of points to consider when interpreting heritability:

  • A high heritability does not mean that the environment has no effect on the development of a trait, or that learning is not involved. Vocabulary size, for example, is very substantially heritable (and highly correlated with general intelligence) although every word in an individual's vocabulary is learned. In a society in which plenty of words are available in everyone's environment, especially for individuals who are motivated to seek them out, the number of words that individuals actually learn depends to a considerable extent on their genetic predispositions.[67]
  • A common error is to assume that because something is heritable it is necessarily unchangeable. As previously noted, heritable traits can depend on learning, and they may be subject to other environmental effects as well. The value of heritability can change if the distribution of environments (or of genes) in the population is substantially altered. For example, an impoverished or suppressive environment could fail to support the development of a trait, and hence restrict individual variation. Differences in variation of heritability are found between developed and developing nations. This could affect estimates of heritability.[67] Another example is Phenylketonuria which previously caused mental retardation for everyone who had this genetic disorder. Today, this can be prevented by following a modified diet.
  • On the other hand, there can be effective environmental changes that do not change heritability at all. If the environment relevant to a given trait improves in a way that affects all members of the population equally, the mean value of the trait will rise without any change in its heritability (because the differences among individuals in the population will stay the same). This has evidently happened for height: the heritability of stature is high, but average heights continue to increase.[67]
  • Even in developed nations, high heritability of a trait within a given group has no necessary implications for the source of a difference between groups.[67][77]

IQ and the brain

In 2004, Richard Haier, professor of psychology in the Department of Pediatrics and colleagues at University of California, Irvine and the University of New Mexico used MRI to obtain structural images of the brain in 47 normal adults who also took standard IQ tests. The study demonstrated that general human intelligence appears to be based on the volume and location of gray matter tissue in the brain, and also demonstrated that, of the brain's gray matter, only about 6 percent appeared to be related to IQ.[78][non-primary source needed]

Many different sources of information have converged on the view that the frontal lobes are critical for fluid intelligence. Patients with damage to the frontal lobe are impaired on fluid intelligence tests (Duncan et al. 1995). The volume of frontal grey (Thompson et al. 2001)[verification needed] and white matter (Schoenemann et al. 2005)[verification needed] have also been associated with general intelligence. In addition, recent neuroimaging studies have limited this association to the lateral prefrontal cortex. Duncan and colleagues (2000) showed using Positron Emission Tomography that problem-solving tasks that correlated more highly with IQ also activate the lateral prefrontal cortex.[non-primary source needed] More recently, Gray and colleagues (2003) used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to show that those individuals that were more adept at resisting distraction on a demanding working memory task had both a higher IQ and increased prefrontal activity. For an extensive review of this topic, see Gray and Thompson (2004).[79]

A study involving 307 children (aged from six to nineteen) measuring the size of brain structures using magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) and measuring verbal and non-verbal abilities has been conducted (Shaw et al. 2006).[80][non-primary source needed] This study indicated that higher intelligence is not directly related to cortical thickness, but rather the change in cortical thickness over time. More intelligent children would develop a thicker cortex initially, and then their cortex would go through a more rigorous thinning process.[81] Garlick (2010) has argued that this reduction in cortical thickness reflects a pruning process of the neural connections, and this pruning process results in a better ability to identify abstractions in the environment.[82][verification needed]

There is "a highly significant association" between the CHRM2 gene and intelligence according to a 2006 Dutch family study. The study concluded that there was an association between the CHRM2 gene on chromosome 7 and Performance IQ, as measured by the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised. The Dutch family study used a sample of 667 individuals from 304 families.[83][non-primary source needed] A similar association was found independently in the Minnesota Twin and Family Study (Comings et al. 2003) and by the Department of Psychiatry at the Washington University.[84][non-primary source needed]

Significant injuries isolated to one side of the brain, especially those occurring at a young age, may not significantly affect IQ.[85]

Studies reach conflicting conclusions regarding the controversial idea that brain size correlates positively with IQ. Jensen and Reed claim no direct correlation exists in nonpathological subjects.[86] A more recent meta-analysis suggests otherwise.[87]

Malleability (mutability or changeability)

Although IQ is believed by many to be immutable,[88] recent research suggests that certain mental activities can change the brain's ability to process information, leading to the conclusion that intelligence can be altered or changed over time. The brain is now properly understood to be neuroplastic and hence far more amenable to change than once was thought. Studies into the neuroscience of animals indicate that challenging activities can produce changes in gene expression patterns of the brain. (Training Degus to Use Rakes [89] and Iriki's earlier research with macaque monkeys indicating brain changes.)

A study on young adults published in April 2008 by a team from the Universities of Michigan and Bern supports the possibility of the transfer of fluid intelligence from specifically designed working memory training.[90] Further research will be needed to determine nature, extent and duration of the proposed transfer:[91] Among other questions, it remains to be seen whether the results extend to other kinds of fluid intelligence tests than the matrix test used in the study, and if so, whether, after training, fluid intelligence measures retain their correlation with educational and occupational achievement or if the value of fluid intelligence for predicting performance on other tasks changes. It is also unclear whether the training is durable of extended periods of time.

The peak capacity for both fluid intelligence and crystallized intelligence is 26 years. This is followed by a slow decline.[92]

Validity and social significance

While IQ is sometimes treated as an end unto itself, scholarly work on IQ focuses to a large extent on IQ's validity, that is, the degree to which IQ correlates with outcomes such as job performance, social pathologies, or academic achievement. Different IQ tests differ in their validity for various outcomes.[citation needed] Traditionally, correlation for IQ and outcomes is viewed as a means to also predict performance; however readers should distinguish between prediction in the hard sciences and the social sciences.

Health

People with a higher IQ have generally lower adult morbidity and mortality. Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder,[93] and schizophrenia[94][95] are less prevalent in higher IQ bands. People in the midsts of a major depressive episode have been shown to have a lower IQ than when without symptoms and lower cognitive ability than people without depression of equivalent verbal intelligence.[96][97]

A study of 11,282 individuals in Scotland who took intelligence tests at ages 7, 9 and 11 in the 1950s and 1960s, found an "inverse linear association" between childhood IQ scores and hospital admissions for injuries in adulthood. The association between childhood IQ and the risk of later injury remained even after accounting for factors such as the child's socioeconomic background.[98] Research in Scotland has also shown that a 15-point lower IQ meant people had a fifth less chance of living to 76, while those with a 30-point disadvantage were 37% less likely than those with a higher IQ to live that long.[99]

A decrease in IQ has also been shown as an early predictor of late-onset Alzheimer's Disease and other forms of dementia. In a 2004 study, Cervilla and colleagues showed that tests of cognitive ability provide useful predictive information up to a decade before the onset of dementia.[100] However, when diagnosing individuals with a higher level of cognitive ability, in this study those with IQs of 120 or more,[101] patients should not be diagnosed from the standard norm but from an adjusted high-IQ norm that measured changes against the individual's higher ability level. In 2000, Whalley and colleagues published a paper in the journal Neurology, which examined links between childhood mental ability and late-onset dementia. The study showed that mental ability scores were significantly lower in children who eventually developed late-onset dementia when compared with other children tested.[102]

Several factors can lead to significant cognitive impairment, particularly if they occur during pregnancy and childhood when the brain is growing and the blood-brain barrier is less effective. Such impairment may sometimes be permanent, or may sometimes be partially or wholly compensated for by later growth. Several harmful factors may also combine, possibly causing greater impairment.

Developed nations have implemented several health policies regarding nutrients and toxins known to influence cognitive function. These include laws requiring fortification of certain food products and laws establishing safe levels of pollutants (e.g. lead, mercury, and organochlorides). Comprehensive policy recommendations targeting reduction of cognitive impairment in children have been proposed.[103]

In terms of the effect of one's intelligence on health, in one British study, high childhood IQ was shown to correlate with one's chance of becoming a vegetarian in adulthood.[104] In another British study, high childhood IQ was shown to inversely correlate with the chances of smoking.[105]

Other tests

One study found a correlation of .82 between g (general intelligence factor) and SAT scores;[106] another has found correlation of .81 between g and GCSE scores.[107]

Correlations between IQ scores (general cognitive ability) and achievement test scores are reported to be .81 by Deary and colleagues, with the percentage of variance accounted for by general cognitive ability ranging "from 58.6% in Mathematics and 48% in English to 18.1% in Art and Design".[107]

School performance

The American Psychological Association's report Intelligence: Knowns and Unknowns[67] states that wherever it has been studied, children with high scores on tests of intelligence tend to learn more of what is taught in school than their lower-scoring peers. The correlation between IQ scores and grades is about .50. However, this means that they explain only 25% of the variance. Achieving good grades depends on many factors other than IQ, such as "persistence, interest in school, and willingness to study" (p. 81).

Job performance

According to Frank Schmidt and John Hunter, "for hiring employees without previous experience in the job the most valid predictor of future performance is general mental ability."[108] The validity of IQ as a predictor of job performance is above zero for all work studied to date, but varies with the type of job and across different studies, ranging from 0.2 to 0.6.[109] While IQ is more strongly correlated with reasoning and less so with motor function [110] IQ-test scores predict performance ratings in all occupations.[108] That said, for highly qualified activities (research, management) low IQ scores are more likely to be a barrier to adequate performance, whereas for minimally-skilled activities, athletic strength (manual strength, speed, stamina, and coordination) are more likely to influence performance.[108] It is largely mediated through the quicker acquisition of job-relevant knowledge that IQ predicts job performance.

In establishing a causal direction to the link between IQ and work performance, longitudinal studies by Watkins and others suggest that IQ exerts a causal influence on future academic achievement, whereas academic achievement does not substantially influence future IQ scores.[111] Treena Eileen Rohde and Lee Anne Thompson write that general cognitive ability but not specific ability scores predict academic achievement, with the exception that processing speed and spatial ability predict performance on the SAT math beyond the effect of general cognitive ability.[112]

The American Psychological Association's report Intelligence: Knowns and Unknowns[67] states that other individual characteristics such as interpersonal skills, aspects of personality etc. are probably of equal or greater importance, but at this point we do not have equally reliable instruments to measure them.[67] More recently, others argue that since most professional tasks are now standardized or automated, and ranked IQ is a stable measurement over time with high correlation with many positive personal traits from the general population, it is the best tool to help determine hiring and job placement at any stage in a career, independently of experience, personality bias or any formal training one may acquire.[citation needed]

Income

Some researchers claim that "in economic terms it appears that the IQ score measures something with decreasing marginal value. It is important to have enough of it, but having lots and lots does not buy you that much."[113][114]

Other studies show that ability and performance for jobs are linearly related, such that at all IQ levels, an increase in IQ translates into a concomitant increase in performance.[115] Charles Murray, coauthor of The Bell Curve, found that IQ has a substantial effect on income independently of family background.[116]

Taking the above two principles together, very high IQ produces very high job performance, but no greater income than slightly high IQ. Studies also show that high IQ is related to higher net worth.[117]

The American Psychological Association's report Intelligence: Knowns and Unknowns[67] states that IQ scores account for about one-fourth of the social status variance and one-sixth of the income variance. Statistical controls for parental SES eliminate about a quarter of this predictive power. Psychometric intelligence appears as only one of a great many factors that influence social outcomes.[67]

Some studies claim that IQ only accounts for a sixth of the variation in income because many studies are based on young adults (many of whom have not yet completed their education). On pg 568 of The g Factor, Arthur Jensen claims that although the correlation between IQ and income averages a moderate 0.4 (one sixth or 16% of the variance), the relationship increases with age, and peaks at middle age when people have reached their maximum career potential. In the book, A Question of Intelligence, Daniel Seligman cites an IQ income correlation of 0.5 (25% of the variance).

A 2002 study[118] further examined the impact of non-IQ factors on income and concluded that an individual's location, inherited wealth, race, and schooling are more important as factors in determining income than IQ.

Other correlations with IQ

In addition, IQ and its correlation to health, violent crime, gross state product, and government effectiveness are the subject of a 2006 paper in the publication Intelligence. The paper breaks down IQ averages by U.S. states using the federal government's National Assessment of Educational Progress math and reading test scores as a source.[119]

There is a correlation of -0.19 between IQ scores and number of juvenile offences in a large Danish sample; with social class controlled, the correlation dropped to -0.17. Similarly, the correlations for most "negative outcome" variables are typically smaller than 0.20, which means that test scores are associated with less than 4% of their total variance. It is important to realize that the causal links between psychometric ability and social outcomes may be indirect. Children with poor scholastic performance may feel alienated. Consequently, they may be more likely to engage in delinquent behavior, compared to other children who do well.[67]

IQ is also negatively correlated with certain diseases.

Tambs et al.[120] found that occupational status, educational attainment, and IQ are individually heritable; and further found that "genetic variance influencing educational attainment ... contributed approximately one-fourth of the genetic variance for occupational status and nearly half the genetic variance for IQ." In a sample of U.S. siblings, Rowe et al.[121] report that the inequality in education and income was predominantly due to genes, with shared environmental factors playing a subordinate role.

In 2008, intelligence researcher Helmuth Nyborg examined whether IQ relates to denomination and income, using representative data from the National Longitudinal Study of Youth. His results demonstrated that on average, Atheists scored 1.95 IQ points higher than Agnostics, 3.82 points higher than Liberal persuasions, and 5.89 IQ points higher than Dogmatic persuasions. [122]

Group differences

Among the most controversial issues related to the study of intelligence is the observation that intelligence measures such as IQ scores vary between ethnic and racial groups and sexes. While there is little scholarly debate about the existence of some of these differences, their causes remain highly controversial both within academia and in the public sphere.

Sex

Men and women have statistically significant differences in average scores on tests of particular abilities.[123][124] Studies also illustrate consistently greater variance in the performance of men compared to that of women.[125]

IQ tests are weighted on these sex differences so there is no bias on average in favor of one sex, however the consistent difference in variance is not removed. Because the tests are defined so there is no average difference it is difficult to put any meaning on a statement that one sex has a higher intelligence than the other. However some people have made claims like this even using unbiased IQ tests. For instance, there are claims that men tend to outperform women on average by three to four IQ points based on tests of medical students where the greater variance of men's IQ can be expected to contribute to the result,[126] or where a 'correction' is made for different maturation ages.[127]

Race

The 1996 Task Force investigation on Intelligence sponsored by the American Psychological Association concluded that there are significant variations in I.Q. across races.[67] The problem of determining the causes underlying this variation relates to the question of the contributions of "nature and nurture" to I.Q. Psychologists such as Alan S. Kaufman[128] and Nathan Brody[129] and statisticians such as Bernie Devlin[130] point out there are insufficient data to conclude that this is because of genetic influences. One of the most notable authors arguing for a strong genetic influence on this score difference is Arthur Jensen. In contrast, Richard Nisbett, the long-time director of the Culture and Cognition program at the University of Michigan, argues that environmental influences are not sufficiently taken into account in Jensen's writings.[131]

Age

For decades, it has been reported in practitioners' handbooks and textbooks on IQ testing that IQ declines with age after the beginning of adulthood. However, later researchers pointed out that this phenomenon is related to the Flynn effect and is in part a cohort effect rather than a true aging effect.

There have been a variety of studies of IQ and aging since the norming of the first Wechsler Intelligence Scale drew attention to IQ differences in different age groups of adults. Current consensus is that fluid intelligence generally declines with age after early adulthood, while crystallized intelligence remains intact. Both cohort effects (the birth year of the test-takers) and practice effects (test-takers taking the same form of IQ test more than once) must be controlled for to gain accurate data. It is unclear whether any lifestyle intervention can preserve fluid intelligence into older ages.[132]

Public policy

In the United States, certain public policies and laws regarding military service,[133] [134] education, public benefits,[135] capital punishment,[136] and employment incorporate an individual's IQ into their decisions. However, in the case of Griggs v. Duke Power Co. in 1971, for the purpose of minimizing employment practices that disparately impacted racial minorities, the U.S. Supreme Court banned the use of IQ tests in employment, except in very rare cases.[137] Internationally, certain public policies, such as improving nutrition and prohibiting neurotoxins, have as one of their goals raising, or preventing a decline in, intelligence.

Criticism and views

Binet

Alfred Binet, a French psychologist, did not believe that IQ test scales qualified to measure intelligence. He neither invented the term "intelligence quotient" nor supported its numerical expression.[citation needed] He stated:

The scale, properly speaking, does not permit the measure of intelligence, because intellectual qualities are not superposable, and therefore cannot be measured as linear surfaces are measured.
—Binet, 1905

Binet had designed the Binet-Simon intelligence scale in order to identify students who needed special help in coping with the school curriculum. He argued that with proper remedial education programs, most students regardless of background could catch up and perform quite well in school. He did not believe that intelligence was a measurable fixed entity.

Binet cautioned:

Some recent thinkers seem to have given their moral support to these deplorable verdicts by affirming that an individual's intelligence is a fixed quantity, a quantity that cannot be increased. We must protest and react against this brutal pessimism; we must try to demonstrate that it is founded on nothing.[138]

The Mismeasure of Man

Some scientists dispute psychometrics entirely. In The Mismeasure of Man, Harvard professor and paleontologist Stephen Jay Gould argued that intelligence tests were based on faulty assumptions and showed their history of being used as the basis for scientific racism. He criticized

…the abstraction of intelligence as a single entity, its location within the brain, its quantification as one number for each individual, and the use of these numbers to rank people in a single series of worthiness, invariably to find that oppressed and disadvantaged groups—races, classes, or sexes—are innately inferior and deserve their status.(pp. 24–25)

He spends much of the book criticizing the concept of IQ, including a historical discussion of how the IQ tests were created and a technical discussion of why g is simply a mathematical artifact. Later editions of the book included criticism of The Bell Curve.

Bernard Davis wrote that while the nonscientific reviews of The Mismeasure of Man were almost uniformly laudatory, the reviews in the scientific journals were almost all highly critical.[139]

Peter H. Schönemann

Psychologist Peter Schönemann has been a persistent critic of IQ, calling it "the IQ myth".[140] He argued that Spearman's g is a flawed theory and the high heritability estimates of IQ are based on false assumptions.[141]

[editEric Turkheimer
]
Relation between IQ and intelligence

According to Dr. C. George Boeree of Shippensburg University, intelligence is a person's capacity to (1) acquire knowledge (i.e. learn and understand), (2) apply knowledge (solve problems), and (3) engage in abstract reasoning. It is the power of one's intellect, and as such is clearly a very important aspect of one's overall well-being. Psychologists have attempted to measure it for well over a century.[citation needed]

IQ is associated with intelligence, but may fail to act as an accurate measure of "intelligence" in its broadest sense. This is partly because IQ tests only examine particular areas embodied by the broadest notion of "intelligence", failing to account for certain areas which are also associated with "intelligence" such as creativity or emotional intelligence.[citation needed]

Several other ways of measuring intelligence have been proposed. Daniel Schacter, Daniel Gilbert, and others have moved beyond general intelligence and IQ as the sole means to describe intelligence.[142]

Test bias

The American Psychological Association's report Intelligence: Knowns and Unknowns[67] states that IQ tests as predictors of social achievement are not biased against people of African descent since they predict future performance, such as school achievement, similarly to the way they predict future performance for people of European descent.[67]

However, IQ tests may well be biased when used in other situations. A 2005 study stated that "differential validity in prediction suggests that the WAIS-R test may contain cultural influences that reduce the validity of the WAIS-R as a measure of cognitive ability for Mexican American students,"[143] indicating a weaker positive correlation relative to sampled white students. Other recent studies have questioned the culture-fairness of IQ tests when used in South Africa.[144][145] Standard intelligence tests, such as the Stanford-Binet, are often inappropriate for children with autism; the alternative of using developmental or adaptive skills measures are relatively poor measures of intelligence in autistic children, and may have resulted in incorrect claims that a majority of children with autism are mentally retarded.[146]

Outdated methodology

A 2006 review article says that contemporary mainstream test analysis does not reflect substantial recent developments in the field and "bears an uncanny resemblance to the psychometric state of the art as it existed in the 1950s."[147]

The view of the American Psychological Association

In response to the controversy surrounding The Bell Curve, the American Psychological Association's Board of Scientific Affairs established a task force in 1995 to write a report on the state of intelligence research which could be used by all sides as a basis for discussion. The full text of the report is available through several websites.[67][148]

In this paper the representatives of the association regret that IQ-related works are frequently written with a view to their political consequences: "research findings were often assessed not so much on their merits or their scientific standing as on their supposed political implications".

The task force concluded that IQ scores do have high predictive validity for individual differences in school achievement. They confirm the predictive validity of IQ for adult occupational status, even when variables such as education and family background have been statistically controlled. They found that individual differences in intelligence are substantially influenced by genetics and that both genes and environment, in complex interplay, are essential to the development of intellectual competence.

The report stated that a number of biological factors, including malnutrition, exposure to toxic substances, and various prenatal and perinatal stressors, result in lowered psychometric intelligence under at least some conditions. The task force agrees that large differences do exist between the average IQ scores of blacks and whites, and that these differences cannot be attributed to biases in test construction. The task force suggests that explanations based on social status and cultural differences are possible, and that environmental factors have raised mean test scores in many populations. Regarding genetic causes, they noted that there is not much direct evidence on this point, but what little there is fails to support the genetic hypothesis.

The APA journal that published the statement, American Psychologist, subsequently published eleven critical responses in January 1997, several of them arguing that the report failed to examine adequately the evidence for partly-genetic explanations.

High IQ societies

There are social organizations, some international, which limit membership to people who have scores as high as or higher than the 98th percentile on some IQ test or equivalent. Mensa International is perhaps the most well known of these. There are other groups requiring a score above the 98th percentile.

Many websites and magazines use the term IQ to refer to technical or popular knowledge in a variety of subjects not related to intelligence, including sex,[149] poker,[150] and American football,[151] among a wide variety of other topics. These tests are generally not standardized and do not fit within the normal definition of intelligence. Intelligence tests such as the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, Stanford-Binet, Woodcock-Johnson III Tests of Cognitive Abilities, or the Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children-II, to name some of the best constructed, are not merely placing a test taker's score within the norm, as presumably are the thousands of alleged "IQ Tests" found on the internet, but they are also testing factors (e.g., fluid and crystallized intelligence, working memory, and the like) that were previously found to represent pure measures of intelligence using factor analysis. This claim may not be made for the hundreds of online tests marketing themselves as IQ Tests, a distinction that may be unfortunately lost upon the public taking them.

Reference charts

IQ reference charts are tables suggested by test publishers to divide intelligence ranges in various categories.

See also

References

Notes

  1. ^ a b Mackintosh, N. J. (1998). IQ and Human Intelligence. Oxford: Oxford University Press. p. 15. ISBN 978-0-19-852367-3. Lay summary (9 August 2010). 
  2. ^ Intelligence: Knowns and Unknowns (Report of a Task Force established by the Board of Scientific Affairs of the American Psychological Association - Released August 7, 1995 — a slightly edited version was published in the American Psychologist, Feb 1996)
  3. ^ Johnson, Wendy; Turkheimer, E.; Gottesman, Irving; Bouchard, Thomas (2009). "Beyond Heritability: Twin Studies in Behavioral Research". Current Directions in Psychological Science 18 (4): 217–220. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01639.x. PMID 20625474. PMC 2899491. http://people.virginia.edu/~ent3c/papers2/Articles%20for%20Online%20CV/Johnson%20%282009%29.pdf. Retrieved 29 June 2010. 
  4. ^ Turkheimer, Eric (spring 2008). "A Better Way to Use Twins for Developmental Research". LIFE Newsletter (Max Planck Institute for Human Development): 2–5. http://people.virginia.edu/~ent3c/papers2/Articles%20for%20Online%20CV/Turkheimer%20%282008%29.pdf. Retrieved 29 June 2010. 
  5. ^ a b c Devlin, B.; Daniels, Michael; Roeder, Kathryn (31 July 1997). "The heritability of IQ". Nature 388 (6641): 468–471. doi:10.1038/41319. PMID 9242404. http://www.psych.umn.edu/courses/spring06/mcguem/psy5137/readings/devlin%201997.pdf. Retrieved 27 August 2010. 
  6. ^ Gillham, N. W. (2001). Sir Francis Galton and the birth of eugenics. Annual Review of Genetics, 35, 83-101.
  7. ^ Galton, F. (1865). Hereditary talent and character. Classics in the history of psychology. Retrieved April 4, 2007
  8. ^ Binet, Alfred (1916) [1905]. "New methods for the diagnosis of the intellectual level of subnormals". The development of intelligence in children: The Binet-Simon Scale. E. S. Kite (Trans.). Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins. pp. 37–90. http://psychclassics.asu.edu/Binet/binet1.htm. Retrieved 10 July 2010. "originally published as Méthodes nouvelles pour le diagnostic du niveau intellectuel des anormaux. L'Année Psychologique, 11, 191–244" 
  9. ^ Wallin, J. E. W. (1911). The new clinical psychology and the psycho-clinicist. Journal of Educational Psychology, 2(4), 191-210.
  10. ^ Spearman, C. (1904). “General intelligence,” objectively determined and measured. American Journal of Psychology, 15(2), 201-293.
  11. ^ a b Mayrhauser, R. T. V. (1992). The mental testing community and validity: A prehistory. American Psychologist, 47(2), 244-253.
  12. ^ Richardson, T. E. (2003). Howard Andrew Knox and the origins of performance testing on Ellis Island, 1212-1216. History of Psychology, 6(2), 143-170.
  13. ^ Terman, L. M., Lyman, G., Ordahl, G., Ordahl, L., Galbreath, N., and Talbert, W. (1915). The Stanford revision of the Binet-Simon scale and some results from its application to 1000 non-selected children. The Journal of Educational Psychology, 6(9), 551-562.
  14. ^ Carmichael, L. (1957). Robert Mearns Yerkes. The Psychological Review, 64(1), 1-7.
  15. ^ a b c Yerkes, R. (1919). Report of the psychology committee of the national research council. The Psychological Review, 26(2), 83-149.
  16. ^ Driskell, J. E., & Olmstead, B. (1989). Psychology and the military: Research applications and trends. American Psychologist, 44(1), 43-54.
  17. ^ a b c d e Kevles, D. J. (1968). Testing the army's intelligence: Psychologists and the military in World War I. The Journal of American History, 55(3), 565-581.
  18. ^ a b Keene, J. D. (1994). Intelligence and morale in the army of a democracy: The genesis of military psychology during the First World War. Military Psychology, 6(4), 235-253.
  19. ^ Kevles, D. J. (1968). Testing the army's intelligence: Psychologists and the military in World War I. The Journal of American History, 55(3), 565-581.
  20. ^ Kennedy, C. H., & McNeil, J. A. (2006). A history of military psychology. In C. H. Kennedy, E. A. Zillmer (Eds.), Military Psychology: Clinical and Operational Applications (pp.1-17). New York: Guilford Publications.
  21. ^ Katzell, R. A., & Austin, J. T. (1992). From then to now: the development of industrial-organizational psychology in the United States. Journal of Applied Psychology. 77(6), 803-835.
  22. ^ Goddard, H. H. (1921). The sub-normal mind versus the abnormal. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 16(1), 47-54.
  23. ^ a b Alper, T. G., & Boring, E. G. (1944). Intelligence-test scores of northern and southern white and negro recruits in 1918. Journal of Abnormal & Social Psychology, 39(4), 471-474.
  24. ^ Thurstone, L. L. (1934). The vectors of mind. Psychological Review, 41, 1-32.
  25. ^ Terman, L. M. (1924). The mental test as a psychological method. The Psychological Review, 31(2), 93-117.
  26. ^ Garrett, H. E. (1945). A note on the intelligence scores of negroes and whites in 1918. Journal of Abnormal & Social Psychology, 40(3), 344-346.
  27. ^ Rembis, M. A. (2004). “I ain't been reading while on parole”: Experts, mental tests, and eugenic commitment law in Illinois, 1890-1940. History of Psychology, 7(3), 225-247.
  28. ^ S.E. Embretson & S.P.Reise: Item response theory for psychologists, 2000. "...for many other psychological tests, normal distributions are achieved by normalizing procedures. For example, intelligence tests..." Found on: http://books.google.se/books?id=rYU7rsi53gQC&pg=PA29&lpg=PA29&dq=%22intelligence+tests%22+normalize&source=bl&ots=ZAIQEgaa6Q&sig=q-amDaZqx7Ix6mMkvIDMnj9M9O0&hl=sv&ei=lEEJTNqSIYWMOPqLuRE&sa=X&oi=book_result&ct=result&resnum=7&ved=0CEIQ6AEwBg#v=onepage&q&f=false
  29. ^ Pinneau, Samuel R. (1961). Changes in Intelligence Quotient Infancy to Maturity: New Insights from the Berkeley Growth Study with Implications for the Stanford-Binet Scales and Applications to Professional Practice. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. 
  30. ^ Mussen, Paul Henry (1973). Psychology: An Introduction. Lexington (MA): Heath. p. 363. ISBN 0-669-61383-7. "The I.Q. is essentially a rank; there are no true "units" of intellectual ability." 
  31. ^ Truch, Steve (1993). The WISC-III Companion: A Guide to Interpretation and Educational Intervention. Austin (TX): Pro-Ed. p. 35. ISBN 0890795851. "An IQ score is not an equal-interval score, as is evident in Table A.4 in the WISC-III manual." 
  32. ^ Bartholomew, David J. (2004). Measuring Intelligence: Facts and Fallacies. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. p. 50. ISBN 9780521544788. Lay summary (27 July 2010). "When we come to quantities like IQ or g, as we are presently able to measure them, we shall see later that we have an even lower level of measurement—an ordinal level. This means that the numbers we assign to individuals can only be used to rank them—the number tells us where the individual comes in the rank order and nothing else." 
  33. ^ Mackintosh, N. J. (1998). IQ and Human Intelligence. Oxford: Oxford University Press. pp. 30–31. ISBN 0-19-852367-X. "In the jargon of psychological measurement theory, IQ is an ordinal scale, where we are simply rank-ordering people. . . . It is not even appropriate to claim that the 10-point difference between IQ scores of 110 and 100 is the same as the 10-point difference between IQs of 160 and 150" 
  34. ^ Perleth, Christoph; Schatz, Tanja; Mönks, Franz J. (2000). "Early Identification of High Ability". In Heller, Kurt A.; Mönks, Franz J.; Sternberg, Robert J. et al.. International Handbook of Giftedness and Talent (2nd ed.). Amsterdam: Pergamon. p. 301. ISBN 978-0-08-043796-5. "norm tables that provide you with such extreme values are constructed on the basis of random extrapolation and smoothing but not on the basis of empirical data of representative samples." 
  35. ^ Terman, Lewis Madison; Merrill, Maude A. (1937). Measuring intelligence: A guide to the administration of the new revised Stanford-Binet tests of intelligence. Riverside textbooks in education. Boston (MA): Houghton Mifflin. "it becomes clear that the probable error of an I.Q. score is not a constant amount, but a variable which increases as I.Q. increases. It has frequently been noted in the literature that gifted subjects show greater I.Q. fluctuation than do clinical cases with low I.Q.'s . . . . we now see that this trend is inherent in the I.Q. technique itself, and might have been predicted on logical grounds." 
  36. ^ Hopkins, Kenneth D.; Stanley, Julian C. (1981). Educational and Psychological Measurement and Evaluation (sixth ed.). Engelwood Cliffs (NJ): Prentice Hall. ISBN 0-13-236273-2. "obtained scores tend to underestimate universe scores for the bottom half of the distribution and overestimate universe scores for the top half of the distribution" 
  37. ^ Hopkins, Kenneth D.; Stanley, Julian C. (1981). Educational and Psychological Measurement and Evaluation (sixth ed.). Engelwood Cliffs (NJ): Prentice Hall. p. 346. ISBN 0-13-236273-2. 
  38. ^ Kaufman, Alan S. (2009). IQ Testing 101. New York: Springer Publishing. pp. 151–153. ISBN 978-0-8261-0629-2. 
  39. ^ Terman, Lewis Madison; Merrill, MaudeA. (1937). Measuring intelligence: A guide to the administration of the new revised Stanford-Binet tests of intelligence. Riverside textbooks in education. Boston (MA): Houghton Mifflin. p. 44. 
  40. ^ Anastasi, Anne; Urbina, Susana (1997). Psychological Testing (Seventh ed.). Upper Saddle River (NJ): Prentice Hall. pp. 326–327. ISBN 978-0023030857. Lay summary (28 July 2010). 
  41. ^ Kaufman, Alan S. (2009). IQ Testing 101. New York: Springer Publishing. pp. 151–153. ISBN 978-0-8261-0629-2. 
  42. ^ a b Neisser U (1997). "Rising Scores on Intelligence Tests". American Scientist 85: 440–7. http://www.americanscientist.org/issues/feature/rising-scores-on-intelligence-tests/1. 
  43. ^ Neisser, Ulric, ed (1998). The Rising Curve: Long-Term Gains in IQ and Related Measures. APA Science Volume Series. Washington (DC): American Psychological Association. ISBN 978-1-55798-503-3. 
  44. ^ a b Mackintosh, N. J. (1998). IQ and Human Intelligence. Oxford: Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-852367-3. Lay summary (9 August 2010). 
  45. ^ Flynn, James R. (2009). What Is Intelligence: Beyond the Flynn Effect (expanded paperback ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-0-521-74147-7. Lay summary (18 July 2010). 
  46. ^ Flynn, James R. title=The Mean IQ of Americans: Massive Gains 1932 to 1978 (1984). Psychological Bulletin 95: 29–51. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.95.1.29. 
  47. ^ Flynn, James R. (March 1987). "Massive IQ Gains in 14 Nations: What IQ Tests Really Measure". Psychological Bulletin 101 (2): 171–191. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.101.2.171. http://psycnet.apa.org/index.cfm?fa=buy.optionToBuy&id=2008-10701-002. Retrieved 22 August 2010. 
  48. ^ Zhou, Xiaobin; Grégoire, Jacques; Zhu, Jianjin (2010). "The Flynn Effect and the Wechsler Scales". In Weiss, Lawrence G.; Saklofske, Donald H.; Coalson, Diane et al.. WAIS-IV Clinical Use and Interpretation: Scientist-Practitioner Perspectives. Practical Resources for the Mental Health Professional. Alan S. Kaufman (Foreword). Amsterdam: Academic Press. ISBN 978-0-12-375035-8. Lay summary (16 August 2010). 
  49. ^ Qian M, Wang D, Watkins WE, et al. (2005). "The effects of iodine on intelligence in children: a meta-analysis of studies conducted in China". Asia Pacific Journal of Clinical Nutrition 14 (1): 32–42. PMID 15734706. 
  50. ^ Gene governs IQ boost from breastfeeding
  51. ^ Caspi A, Williams B, Kim-Cohen J, et al. (2007). "Moderation of breastfeeding effects on the IQ by genetic variation in fatty acid metabolism". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 104 (47): 18860. doi:10.1073/pnas.0704292104. PMID 17984066. 
  52. ^ Schellenberg, E. G. (2004). "Music lessons enhance IQ." Psychol Sci 15(8): 511-4.
  53. ^ (Klingberg et al., 2002)
  54. ^ Jaeggi SM, Buschkuehl M, Jonides J, Perrig WJ (2008). "Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory". Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 105 (19): 6829–6833. doi:10.1073/pnas.0801268105. PMID 18443283. PMC 2383929. http://www.pnas.org/cgi/content/abstract/0801268105v1. 
  55. ^ a b Plomin et al. (2001, 2003)
  56. ^ Harris (1998)
  57. ^ Bouchard TJ (1998). "Genetic and environmental influences on adult intelligence and special mental abilities". Hum. Biol. 70 (2): 257–279. PMID 9549239. 
  58. ^ a b Stoolmiller M (1999). "Implications of the restricted range of family environments for estimates of heritability and nonshared environment in behavior-genetic adoption studies". Psychol Bull 125 (4): 392–409. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.125.4.392. PMID 10414224. http://content.apa.org/journals/bul/125/4/392. 
  59. ^ a b McGue, M. et al. (2007). "The Environments of Adopted and Non-adopted Youth: Evidence on Range Restriction From the Sibling Interaction and Behavior Study (SIBS)". Behavior Genetics 37 (3): 449. doi:10.1007/s10519-007-9142-7. PMID 17279339. 
  60. ^ Turkheimer E, Haley A, Waldron M, D'Onofrio B, Gottesman II (November 2003). "Socioeconomic status modifies heritability of IQ in young children". Psychol Sci 14 (6): 623–8. doi:10.1046/j.0956-7976.2003.psci_1475.x. PMID 14629696. 
  61. ^ Socioeconomic status modifies heritability of iq in young children Eric Turkheimer, Andreana Haley, Mary Waldron, Brian D'Onofrio, Irving I. Gottesman. Psychological Science 14 (6), 623–628. 2003
  62. ^ New Thinking on Children, Poverty & IQ November 10, 2003 Connect for Kids
  63. ^ Bouchard, T.J.; McGue, M. (2003). "Genetic and environmental influences on human psychological differences". Journal of Neurobiology 54 (1): 4. doi:10.1002/neu.10160. PMID 12486697. 
  64. ^ a b Dickens and Flynn (2001)
  65. ^ William T. Dickens and James R. Flynn, "The IQ Paradox: Still Resolved," Psychological Review 109, no. 4 (2002).
  66. ^ Plomin R, Pedersen NL, Lichtenstein P, McClearn GE (May 1994). "Variability and stability in cognitive abilities are largely genetic later in life". Behavior Genetics 24 (3): 207–15. doi:10.1007/BF01067188. PMID 7945151. 
  67. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l m n o Neisser et al. (August 7, 1995). "Intelligence: Knowns and Unknowns". Board of Scientific Affairs of the American Psychological Association. http://www.lrainc.com/swtaboo/taboos/apa_01.html. Retrieved August 6, 2006. 
  68. ^ Bouchard TJ, Lykken DT, McGue M, Segal NL, Tellegen A (1990). "Sources of human psychological differences: the Minnesota Study of Twins Reared Apart". Science 250 (4978): 223–228. doi:10.1126/science.2218526. PMID 2218526. http://www.sciencemag.org/cgi/pmidlookup?view=long&pmid=2218526. 
  69. ^ "Commentary: The analysis of variance and the social complexities of genetic causation" by Jeremy Freese (2006) International Journal of Epidemiology 35(3):534-536 doi:10.1093/ije/dyl065
  70. ^ "Commentary: Heritability estimates—long past their sell-by date" by Steven P R Rose (2006) Int. J. Epidemiol. 35: 525-527; doi:10.1093/ije/dyl064
  71. ^ "Commentary: The analysis of variance is an analysis of causes (of a very circumscribed kind)" (2006) Int. J. Epidemiol. 35: 527-531; doi:10.1093/ije/dyl063
  72. ^ "Commentary: The attainability of causal knowledge of genetic effects in complex human traits" (2006) Int. J. Epidemiol. 35: 531-534; doi:10.1093/ije/dyl066
  73. ^ "Commentary: Statistical analysis or biological analysis as tools for understanding biological causes" (2006) Int. J. Epidemiol. '35:' 536-537; doi:10.1093/ije/dyl070
  74. ^ "The analysis of variance and the analysis of causes" (2006) Int. J. Epidemiol. 35: 520-525; doi:10.1093/ije/dyl062
  75. ^ Turkheimer E, Haley A, Waldron M, D'Onofrio B, Gottesman II (November 2003). "Socioeconomic status modifies heritability of IQ in young children". Psychological Science 14 (6): 623–8. doi:10.1046/j.0956-7976.2003.psci_1475.x. PMID 14629696. 
  76. ^ a b Bouchard, TJ (2004). "Genetic influence on human psychological traits - A survey". Current Directions in Psychological Science 13 (4): 148–151. doi:10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.00295.x. 
  77. ^ See: Ethnic Differences in Children's Intelligence Test Scores: Role of Economic Deprivation, Home Environment, and Maternal Characteristics
  78. ^ "Human Intelligence Determined by Volume and Location of Gray Matter Tissue in Brain". Brain Research Institute, UC Irvine College of Medicine. Retrieved August 6, 2006. 
  79. ^ Jeremy R. Gray, Psychology Department, Yale University, and Paul M. Thompson, Laboratory of Nero Imaging, Department of Neurology, University of California, Los Angeles School of Medicine (2004). "Neurobiology of Intelligence: Science and Ethics" (PDF). Nature Publishing Group, Volume 5. http://www.loni.ucla.edu/~thompson/PDF/nrn0604-GrayThompson.pdf. Retrieved 2006-08-06. 
  80. ^ Shaw P, Greenstein D, Lerch J, et al. (2006). "Intellectual ability and cortical development in children and adolescents". Nature 440 (7084): 676–679. doi:10.1038/nature04513. PMID 16572172. 
  81. ^ Nicholas Wade (2006). "Scans Show Different Growth for Intelligent Brains". Brain Research Institute, UCLA... Retrieved 2006-08-06. 
  82. ^ Garlick, D. (2010). Intelligence and the Brain: Solving the mystery of why people differ in IQ and how a child can be a genius. Burbank, CA: Aesop Press. ISBN 978-0615319216. Lay summary. 
  83. ^ Gosso MF, van Belzen M, de Geus EJ, et al. (2006). "Association between the CHRM2 gene and intelligence in a sample of 304 Dutch families". Genes, Brain and Behavior 5 (8): 577–584. doi:10.1111/j.1601-183X.2006.00211.x. PMID 17081262. 
  84. ^ Dick DM, Aliev F, Kramer J, et al. (2007). "Association of CHRM2 with IQ: converging evidence for a gene influencing intelligence". Behav. Genet. 37 (2): 265–272. doi:10.1007/s10519-006-9131-2. PMID 17160701. 
  85. ^ Bava S, Ballantyne AO, Trauner DA (2005). "Disparity of verbal and performance IQ following early bilateral brain damage". Cogn Behav Neurol 18 (3): 163–170. doi:10.1097/01.wnn.0000178228.61938.3e. PMID 16175020. http://meta.wkhealth.com/pt/pt-core/template-journal/lwwgateway/media/landingpage.htm?issn=1543-3633&volume=18&issue=3&spage=163. 
  86. ^ Reed, T.E., & Jensen, A.R. 1993. Cranial capacity: new Caucasian data and comments on Rushton's claimed Mongoloid-Caucasoid brain-size differences. Intelligence, 17, 423-431
  87. ^ McDaniel, M.A. (2005) Big-brained people are smarter: A meta-analysis of the relationship between in vivo brain volume and intelligence. Intelligence, 33, 337-346. PDF
  88. ^ Kaufman, Alan S. (2009). IQ Testing 101. New York: Springer Publishing. pp. 220–222. ISBN 978-0-8261-0629-2. Lay summary (10 August 2010). 
  89. ^ Okanoya K, Tokimoto N, Kumazawa N, Hihara S, Iriki A (2008). "Tool-use training in a species of rodent: the emergence of an optimal motor strategy and functional understanding". PLoS ONE 3 (3): e1860. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0001860. PMID 18365015. 
  90. ^ Jaeggi SM, Buschkuehl M, Jonides J, Perrig WJ (2008). "Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory". Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 105 (19): 6829–6833. doi:10.1073/pnas.0801268105. PMID 18443283. 
  91. ^ R.J. Sternberg (2008). "Increasing fluid intelligence is possible after all (Commentary)". Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 105 (19): 6791–6792. doi:10.1073/pnas.0803396105. PMID 18474863. 
  92. ^ McArdle J J, Ferrer-Caja E, Hamagami F, Woodcock RW. (2002). Comparative longitudinal structural analyses of the growth and decline of multiple intellectual abilities over the life span. Dev. Psychol. 38: 115–142. PMID 11806695 Based on a sample of 1200 US individuals aged 2–95 years representative of both genders of major ethnic groups and all education levels
  93. ^ Breslau N, Lucia VC, Alvarado GF (2006). "Intelligence and other predisposing factors in exposure to trauma and posttraumatic stress disorder: a follow-up study at age 17 years". Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 63 (11): 1238–1245. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.63.11.1238. PMID 17088504. 
  94. ^ Zinkstok JR, de Wilde O, van Amelsvoort TA, Tanck MW, Baas F, Linszen DH (2007). "Association between the DTNBP1 gene and intelligence: a case-control study in young patients with schizophrenia and related disorders and unaffected siblings". Behav Brain Funct 3: 19. doi:10.1186/1744-9081-3-19. PMID 17445278. PMC 1864987. http://www.behavioralandbrainfunctions.com/content/3/1/19. 
  95. ^ Woodberry KA, Giuliano AJ, Seidman LJ (2008). "Premorbid IQ in schizophrenia: a meta-analytic review". Am J Psychiatry 165 (5): 579–587. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.07081242. PMID 18413704. 
  96. ^ Sackeim HA, Freeman J, McElhiney M, Coleman E, Prudic J, Devanand DP (1992). "Effects of major depression on estimates of intelligence". J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 14 (2): 268–288. doi:10.1080/01688639208402828. PMID 1572949. 
  97. ^ Mandelli L, Serretti A, Colombo C, et al. (2006). "Improvement of cognitive functioning in mood disorder patients with depressive symptomatic recovery during treatment: an exploratory analysis". Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 60 (5): 598–604. doi:10.1111/j.1440-1819.2006.01564.x. PMID 16958944. http://www.blackwell-synergy.com/doi/abs/10.1111/j.1440-1819.2006.01564.x. 
  98. ^ Debbie A. Lawlor, University of Bristol, Heather Clark, University of Aberdeen, David A. Leon, London School of Hygiene & Tropical Medicine (2006). "Associations Between Childhood Intelligence and Hospital Admissions for Unintentional Injuries in Adulthood: The Aberdeen Children of the 1950s Cohort Study". American Journal of Public Health, December 2006. http://www.ajph.org/cgi/content/abstract/AJPH.2005.080168v1. Retrieved January 10, 2007. 
  99. ^ Whalley LJ, Deary IJ (2001). "Longitudinal cohort study of childhood IQ and survival up to age 76". BMJ 322 (7290): 819. doi:10.1136/bmj.322.7290.819. PMID 11290633. PMC 30556. http://bmj.com/cgi/pmidlookup?view=long&pmid=11290633. 
  100. ^ Cervilla J, Prince M, Joels S, Lovestone S, Mann A (August 2004). "Premorbid cognitive testing predicts the onset of dementia and Alzheimer's disease better than and independently of APOE genotype". Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 75 (8): 1100–6. doi:10.1136/jnnp.2003.028076. PMID 15258208. 
  101. ^ Dorene Rentz, Brigham and Women's Hospital's Department of Neurology and Harvard Medical School. "More Sensitive Test Norms Better Predict Who Might Develop Alzheimer's Disease". Neuropsychology, published by the American Psychological Association. http://laboratory-manager.advanceweb.com/common/editorial/editorial.aspx?CC=27318. Retrieved 2006-08-06. 
  102. ^ Whalley LJ, Starr JM, Athawes R, Hunter D, Pattie A, Deary IJ (2000). "Childhood mental ability and dementia". Neurology 55 (10): 1455–1459. PMID 11094097. http://www.neurology.org/cgi/pmidlookup?view=long&pmid=11094097. 
  103. ^ Olness K (2003). "Effects on brain development leading to cognitive impairment: a worldwide epidemic". J Dev Behav Pediatr 24 (2): 120–130. PMID 12692458. http://meta.wkhealth.com/pt/pt-core/template-journal/lwwgateway/media/landingpage.htm?issn=0196-206X&volume=24&issue=2&spage=120. 
  104. ^ Gale CR, Deary IJ, Schoon I, Batty GD (February 2007). "IQ in childhood and vegetarianism in adulthood: 1970 British cohort study". BMJ 334 (7587): 245. doi:10.1136/bmj.39030.675069.55. PMID 17175567. 
  105. ^ Taylor, MD; Hart, Carole L.; Smith, George Davey; Starr, John M.; Hole, David J.; Whalley, Lawrence J.; Wilson, Valerie; Deary, Ian J. (2005). "Childhood IQ and social factors on smoking behaviour, lung function and smoking-related outcomes in adulthood: linking the Scottish Mental Survey 1932 and the Midspan studies". British Journal of Health Psychology 10 (3): 399–401. doi:10.1348/135910705X25075. PMID 16238855. 
  106. ^ Frey MC, Detterman DK (June 2004). "Scholastic Assessment or g? The relationship between the Scholastic Assessment Test and general cognitive ability". Psychological Science 15 (6): 373–8. doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00687.x. PMID 15147489. 
  107. ^ a b Deary, I; Strand, S; Smith, P; Fernandes, C (2007). "Intelligence and educational achievement". Intelligence 35: 13. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2006.02.001. 
  108. ^ a b c Schmidt, Frank L.; Hunter, John E. (1998). "The validity and utility of selection methods in personnel psychology: Practical and theoretical implications of 85 years of research findings.". Psychological Bulletin 124: 262. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.124.2.262. 
  109. ^ Hunter, J. E. and Hunter, R. F. (1984). Validity and utility of alternative predictors of job performance. Psychological Bulletin, 96, 72–98.
  110. ^ Warner MH, Ernst J, Townes BD, Peel J, Preston M (1987). "Relationships between IQ and neuropsychological measures in neuropsychiatric populations: within-laboratory and cross-cultural replications using WAIS and WAIS-R". J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 9 (5): 545–562. doi:10.1080/01688638708410768. PMID 3667899. 
  111. ^ Marley W. Watkins, Pui-Wa Lei and Gary L. Canivez. (2007). Psychometric intelligence and achievement: A cross-lagged panel analysis, Intelligence, 35, 59-68.
  112. ^ Treena Eileen Rohde and Lee Anne Thompson. (2007). Predicting academic achievement with cognitive ability, Intelligence, 35, 83-92.
  113. ^ Detterman and Daniel, 1989.
  114. ^ Earl Hunt. "The Role of Intelligence in Modern Society (July-Aug, 1995)". American Scientist. pp. 4 (Nonlinearities in Intelligence). Archived from the original on May 21, 2006. http://web.archive.org/web/20060521185816/http://www.americanscientist.org/template/AssetDetail/assetid/24538/page/4. 
  115. ^ Coward, W.M. and Sackett, P.R. (1990). Linearity of ability-performance relationships: A reconfirmation. Journal of Applied Psychology, 75:297–300.
  116. ^ Murray, Charles (1998). Income Inequality and IQ, AEI Press PDF
  117. ^ Henderson, Mark (April 25, 2007). "Brains dont make you rich IQ study finds". The Times (London). http://www.timesonline.co.uk/tol/uk/science/article1701377.ece. Retrieved May 5, 2010. 
  118. ^ The Inheritance of Inequality Bowles, Samuel; Gintis, Herbert. The Journal of Economic Perspectives. Volume 16, Number 3, 1 August 2002, pp. 3-30(28)
  119. ^ Michael A. McDaniel, Virginia Commonwealth University (accepted for publication August 2006). "Estimating state IQ: Measurement challenges and preliminary correlates" (PDF). Intelligence. http://www.people.vcu.edu/~mamcdani/Publications/McDaniel%20(2006)%20Estimating%20state%20IQ.pdf. 
  120. ^ Tambs K, Sundet JM, Magnus P, Berg K. "Genetic and environmental contributions to the covariance between occupational status, educational attainment, and IQ: a study of twins." Behav Genet. 1989 Mar;19(2):209–22. PMID 2719624.
  121. ^ Rowe, D. C., W. J. Vesterdal, and J. L. Rodgers, "The Bell Curve Revisited: How Genes and Shared Environment Mediate IQ-SES Associations," University of Arizona, 1997
  122. ^ Nyborg, Helmuth (2008-03). "The intelligence–religiosity nexus: A representative study of white adolescent Americans". doi:doi:10.1016/j.intell.2008.08.003. http://www.sciencedirect.com/science?_ob=ArticleURL&_udi=B6W4M-4TFV93D-1&_user=10&_rdoc=1&_fmt=&_orig=search&_sort=d&view=c&_acct=C000050221&_version=1&_urlVersion=0&_userid=10&md5=db2ee09bae0195cc1ecbd026da77245c. Retrieved 2008-10-17. 
  123. ^ Douglas N. Jackson and J. Philippe Rushton, Males have greater g: Sex differences in general mental ability from 100,000 17- to 18-year-olds on the Scholastic Assessment Test, Intelligence, Volume 34, Issue 5, September–October 2006, Pages 479-486.
  124. ^ Lynn, R., & Irwing, P. (2004). Sex differences on the Progressive Matrices: A meta-analysis. Intelligence, 32, 481-498
  125. ^ Deary, I.J., Irwing, P., Der, G., & Bates, T.C. (2007). "Brother–sister differences in the g factor in intelligence: Analysis of full, opposite-sex siblings from the NLSY1979." Intelligence, 35(5): 451-456.
  126. ^ Stumpf, H. and Jackson, D. N. (1994). "Gender-related differences in cognitive abilities: evidence from a medical school admissions program". Personality and Individual Differences 17: 335–344. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(94)90281-X. 
  127. ^ Publications
  128. ^ Kaufman, Alan S. (2009). IQ Testing 101. New York: Springer Publishing. p. 173. ISBN 978-0-8261-0629-2. Lay summary (10 August 2010). 
  129. ^ Brody, Nathan chapter=To g or Not to g—That Is the Question (2005). Wilhelm, Oliver & Engle, Randall W. (Eds.). ed. Handbook of Understanding and Measuring Intelligence. Thousand Oaks (CA): SAGE Publications. 
  130. ^ Bernie Devlin, Stephen E. Fienberg, Daniel P. Resnick & Kathryn Roeder, ed (1997). Intelligence, Genes, and Success: Scientists Respond to the Bell Curve. New York (NY): Springer Verlag. ISBN 0-38798234-5. 
  131. ^ Nisbett, Richard E. (2009). Intelligence and How to Get It: Why Schools and Cultures Count. New York (NY): W. W. Norton. ISBN 978-0-393-06505-3. Lay summary (28 June 2010). 
  132. ^ Kaufman, Alan S. (2009). IQ Testing 101. New York: Springer Publishing. Chapter 8. ISBN 978-0-8261-0629-2. Lay summary (10 August 2010). 
  133. ^ "RAND_TR193.pdf" (PDF). http://www.rand.org/pubs/technical_reports/2005/RAND_TR193.pdf. 
  134. ^ "MR818.ch2.pdf" (PDF). http://www.rand.org/pubs/monograph_reports/MR818/MR818.ch2.pdf. 
  135. ^ "Social Security Administration". http://www.ssa.gov/disability/professionals/bluebook/12.00-MentalDisorders-Adult.htm. 
  136. ^ Flynn, James R. (2009). What Is Intelligence: Beyond the Flynn Effect (expanded paperback ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-0-521-74147-7. Lay summary (18 July 2010). 
  137. ^ Nicholas Lemann. The IQ Meritocracy. Time 100 link
  138. The Return of Determinism?
  139. ^ Davis, Bernard D. (1983). Neo-Lysenkoism, IQ, and the press. The Public Interest, 74, 41-59.
  140. ^ Models and muddles of heritability. Genetica, 99, 97-108
  141. ^ Schönemann, P. H. (1983). Do IQ tests really measure intelligence? Commentary, The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 6, 311-313
  142. ^ The Waning of I.Q. by David Brooks, The New York Times
  143. ^ Culture-Fair Cognitive Ability Assessment Steven P. Verney Assessment, Vol. 12, No. 3, 303-319 (2005)
  144. ^ Shuttleworth-Edwards AB, Kemp RD, Rust AL, Muirhead JG, Hartman NP, Radloff SE (2004). "Cross-cultural effects on IQ test performance: a review and preliminary normative indications on WAIS-III test performance". J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 26 (7): 903–920. doi:10.1080/13803390490510824. PMID 15742541. 
  145. ^ Case for Non-Biased Intelligence Testing Against Black Africans Has Not Been Made: A Comment on Rushton, Skuy, and Bons (2004) 1*, Leah K. Hamilton1, Betty R. Onyura1 and Andrew S. Winston International Journal of Selection and Assessment Volume 14 Issue 3 Page 278 - September 2006
  146. ^ Edelson, MG (2006). "Are the majority of children with autism mentally retarded? a systematic evaluation of the data". Focus Autism Other Dev Disabl 21 (2): 66–83. doi:10.1177/10883576060210020301. http://www.willamette.edu/dept/comm/reprint/edelson/. Retrieved 2007-04-15. 
  147. ^ Borsboom, Denny (September 2006). "The attack of the psychometricians". Psychometrika 71 (3): 425–440. doi:10.1007/s11336-006-1447-6. PMID 19946599. PMC 2779444. http://www.springerlink.com/content/t650617564233751/fulltext.pdf. Retrieved 27 August 2010. 
  148. ^ [1]
  149. ^ "Planned Parenthood Sex IQ". Archived from the original on 2008-07-06. http://web.archive.org/web/20080706120025/http://www.plannedparenthood.org/health-topics/sex-101/your-sex-iq-4328.htm. Retrieved 2008-08-10. 
  150. ^ "NL Holdem Poker IQ Test". http://www.testyourpoker.com. Retrieved 2008-08-10. 
  151. ^ "American Football IQ". http://www.footballiqtest.com. Retrieved 2008-08-10. 

Bibliography

Additional Resources

As advocates of mental health and wellness, we take great pride in educating our readers on the various online therapy providers available. MentalHelp has partnered with several thought leaders in the mental health and wellness space, so we can help you make informed decisions on your wellness journey. MentalHelp may receive marketing compensation from these companies should you choose to use their services.

MentalHelp may receive marketing compensation from the above-listed companies should you choose to use their services.

Ad